In: Psychology
What kinds of perceptual and cognitive processing is needed for understanding spoken and written language? What neural structures are involved?
THE HUMAN BRAIN:
The brain is divided into two halves, a left hemisphere and a right hemisphere. This is called lateralization.In human beings, it is the left hemisphere that usually contains the specialized language areas. While this holds true for 97% of right-handed people, about 19% of left-handed people have their language areas in the right hemisphere and as many as 68% of them have some language abilities in both the left and the right hemispheres.
WHAT IS LANGUAGE PROCESSING?
Language processing refers to the way humans use words to
communicate ideas and feelings, and how such communications are
processed and understood. Thus it is how the brain creates and
understands language.
UNDERSTANDING SPOKEN
LANGUAGE :
Sound waves received by the ear are turned into neural activity by
a complex mechanism involving the eardrum, the bones in the middle
ear, and the hair cells within the cochlea. The auditory nerve
carries the signal from the ears to the brainstem, from where it
passes via the thalamus to the auditory areas of the cerebral
cortex. In the cortex, speech sounds are extracted from the
incoming signal. There are neural circuits in the auditory cortex
that are specialized for speech and language as opposed to other
types of sound.
Speech production and speech perception takes place predominantly in one hemisphere of the brain, usually the left. Several areas within the left hemisphere are involved. Broca’s area, in the frontal lobe, seems to be crucial for syntactic operations in both production and perception of speech. Wernicke’s area, in the temporal lobe, seems to be crucial for accessing the concrete meanings of words. The evidence for the distinction in function between posterior and anterior language areas comes from the study of aphasia, that is, difficulties with language resulting from brain injury.
PERCEPTUAL
PROCESSES
INVOLVED
Speech
perception
Acoustic stimuli are received by the auditive organ and are
converted to bioelectric signals on the organ of Corti. These
electric impulses are then transported through scarpa's ganglion
(vestibulocochlear nerve) to the primary auditory cortex, on both
hemispheres. Each hemisphere treats it differently, nevertheless:
while the left side recognizes distinctive parts such as phonemes,
the right side takes over prosodic characteristics and melodic
information.
The signal is then transported to Wernicke's area on the left
hemisphere (the information that was being processed on the right
hemisphere is able to cross through inter-hemispheric axons), where
the already noted analysis takes part.
During speech comprehension, activations are focused in and around
Wernicke's area. A large body of evidence supports a role for the
posterior superior temporal gyrus in acoustic–phonetic aspects of
speech processing, whereas more ventral sites such as the posterior
middle temporal gyrus (pMTG) are thought to play a higher
linguistic role linking the auditory word form to broadly
distributed semantic knowledge.
Speech
production
From Wernicke's area, the signal is taken to Broca's area through
the arcuate fasciculus. Speech production activations begin prior
to verbal response in the peri-Rolandic cortices (pre- and
postcentral gyri). The role of ventral peri-Rolandic cortices in
speech motor functions has long been appreciated (Broca's area).
The superior portion of the ventral premotor cortex also exhibited
auditory responses preferential to speech stimuli and are part of
the dorsal stream.
Involvement of Wernicke's area in speech production has been
suggested and recent studies document the participation of
traditional Wernicke's area (mid-to posterior superior temporal
gyrus) only in post-response auditory feedback, while demonstrating
a clear pre-response activation from the nearby temporal-parietal
junction (TPJ).
It is believed that the common route to speech production is
through verbal and phonological working memory using the same
dorsal stream areas (temporal-parietal junction, sPMv) implicated
in speech perception and phonological working memory. The observed
pre-response activations at these dorsal stream sites are suggested
to subserve phonological encoding and its translation to the
articulatory score for speech. Post-response Wernicke's
activations, on the other hand, are involved strictly in auditory
self-monitoring.
COGNITIVE
PROCESSES
INVOLVED
Listening
The acoustic speech signal provides a nearly-continuous stream of
spectral information about what the speaker’s mouth is doing (and
indirectly, about the speaker’s intentions). The signal is broken
by brief periods of silence that correspond to closures of the
mouth (during stop consonants) and not generally to breaks between
words. A great deal is known about how the acoustic properties of
the speech waveform are mapped into the perception of phonological
segments and about the recognition of spoken words.The recognition
of words in normal speech has to take place quickly and
efficiently. Estimates of normal speaking rate range from 120 to
200 words per minute, or 120 to 600 syllables, or in some
estimates, as many as 20 to 30 segments per second.One of the
important tasks for a listener is to segment the continuous speech
stream into words. While there are some signals to word beginnings
in the speech streammuch of the work of segmentation must be based
on knowledge of the words that exist in a language. A listener is
generally able to take a string of segments that could be broken at
different points into possible words, and parse it into a coherent
string of words in which every segment is assigned to a word .
UNDERSTANDING WRITTEN LANGUAGE:
PERCEPTUAL
PROCESSES
INVOLVED
The primary visual cortex decodes visual information from printed
words, sentences, and text. Neuroimaging studies of lexical tasks
have also revealed the importance of the fusiform gyrus for lexical
processing in writing systems of several languages, including
alphabets and logographs.The VWFA is largely implicated in mapping
visual information to meaning or retrieval of meaning ,especially
in reading ideograms .It processes information of fine-grained
visual form such as (but not exclusively) information required for
discriminating between words and for combining visual and verbal
linguistic information. Lesions to the VWFA are associated with
impairments in oral reading and oral naming tasks
COGNITIVE
PROCESSES
INVOLVED
Reading
During reading, the visual properties of the text are encoded via a
series of eye movements generally from left-to-right across the
line of text. The visual information is encoded during fixations,
which typically last about 200-250 ms (though the range is from 50
ms to over 500 ms). The movements of the eyes between the
fixations, saccades, typically last 20-30 ms; no new information is
obtained during these movements. Indeed, vision is suppressed
during saccades . On average, the eyes move 7-8 letter spaces
(though the range is from a single letter space to over 20 spaces)
for readers of English and other alphabetic writing systems; letter
spaces, rather than visual angle, are the appropriate measure for
indexing how far the eyes move during reading On about 10-15% of
the saccades, regressions, readers move their eyes backwards in the
text to look at material that has received some prior processing.
As text difficulty increases, readers tend to increase fixation
durations, decrease saccade size, and increase regressions.
The reason readers need to make eye movements during reading is
very much related to the anatomy of the retina.
The cognitive operations involved in writing may be divided for
convenience into central and peripheral processes.
Central processes include semantic, syntactic and other
sentence-level operations, along with those processes responsible
for either retrieving from memory the spelling of a familiar word
or assembling from sound a plausible spelling for an unfamiliar
word or nonword. The end-product of these central processes is an
abstract graphemic representation of words as letter strings.
Peripheral writing processes translate that abstract graphemic
representation into a range of possible output modes, including
handwriting, typing, and spelling aloud. A model of peripheral
writing processes is presented, concentrating on the processes
responsible for creating handwritten output. A number of different
peripheral acquired dysgraphias including mirror writing are then
reviewed and interpreted in terms of impairment at different levels
in the conversion of abstract graphemes into written letters and
words.
NEURAL
STRUCTURES
INVOLVED
IN LANGUAGE
PROCESSING
:
Much of the language function is processed in several association
areas, and there are two well-identified areas that are considered
vital for human communication: Wernicke's area and Broca's area.
These areas are usually located in the dominant hemisphere (the
left hemisphere in 97% of people) and are considered the most
important areas for language processing. This is why language is
considered a localized and lateralized function.the non-dominant
hemisphere also participates in this cognitive function, and there
is ongoing debate on the level of participation of the
less-dominant areas. The non-dominant hemisphere may be
particularly involved in processing the prosody of spoken
language.
There are several areas of the brain that play a critical role in
speech and language.
Broca’s area, located in the left
hemisphere, is associated with speech production and articulation.
Our ability to articulate ideas, as well as use words accurately in
spoken and written language, has been attributed to this crucial
area.
Wernicke’s area is a critical language area in the
posterior superior temporal lobe connects to Broca’s area via a
neural pathway. Wernicke's area is primarily involved in the
comprehension. Historically, this area has been associated with
language processing, whether it is written or spoken.
The angular gyrus allows us to associate multiple
types of language-related information whether auditory, visual or
sensory. It is located in close proximity to other critical brain
regions such as the parietal lobe which processes tactile
sensation, the occipital lobe which is involved in visual analyses
and the temporal lobe which processes sounds. The angular gyrus
allows us to associate a perceived word with different images,
sensations and ideas.
Other Areas
Despite the fact that Broca's and Wernicke's Areas are in different
lobes, they are actually quite near each other and intimately
connected by a tract of nerves called the arcuate
fascilicus. There are also people who have damage
to the arcuate fascilicus, which results in an aphasia known as
conduction aphasia. These people have it a bit better than other
aphasias: They can understand speech, and they can (although with
difficulty) produce coherent speech, they cannot repeat words or
sentences that they hear.