Question

In: Psychology

8. It is said that almost all individuals with mood disorders are also anxious, but not...

8. It is said that almost all individuals with mood disorders are also anxious, but not all those with anxiety disorders show depressed symptoms. What features do the Mood Disorders share with Anxiety disorders? Describe the characteristics that distinguish these two classes of disorders. How do these tow groups differ in their etiology? Their treatment?

Solutions

Expert Solution

DSM-5 provides a somewhat divisive starting point for looking at the overlap between major depression, anxiety disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD). This is because it proposes a separation between anxiety disorders and OCD by placing them in separate chapters of the North American “diagnostic bible.”

In previous versions, they had been united as anxiety disorders. The obsessive-compulsive disorders include OCD itself, body dysmorphic disorder, hoarding disorder, trichotillomania, and excoriation disorder. The anxiety disorders include generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), specific phobia, social phobia, agoraphobia, and panic disorder. These two major groups are in turn separated from the trauma and stressor-related disorders, and of course, mood disorder.

The diagnostic tradition in medicine has always been divided between those who have been called the “lumpers” and those called the “splitters”; in other words, experts driven by their recognition of the similarities between diagnoses and those driven by the differences.

Since diagnosis and classification more generally are a preliminary to more profound understanding of disease, neither is wholly right (nor wholly wrong) and, as the DSM-5 version illustrates, the emphasis can change.

Therefore, a diagnosis of major depression, an anxiety disorder, or OCD may make perfect sense in terms of the primary symptoms of which the patient complains, and on which a differentiated diagnosis is based, yet there may well be a common experience of anxiety and even dysphoria across the conditions, and of course recourse to the same drug or choice of drugs for treatment. In addressing why this overlap occurs, a common neurobiology seems the most obvious explanation.

Lumping: the evidence

DSM-5 itself advocates consideration of shared neural substrate, family traits, genetic risk factors, specific environmental risk factors, biomarkers, shared temperament, abnormalities of emotional processing, symptom similarity, course of illness, high comorbidity, and shared treatment response for confirming relationships between diseases.

In fact, this use of more numerous and more global factors leads to the lumping idea of internalizing disorders on the one hand (into which all the disorders here fall) and externalizing disorders on the other (characterized by aggression, anger outbursts, law-breaking, or hyperactivity). The introduction of this more dimensional approach to diagnosis in DSM-5 also takes note of the likely advantages for bridging to neurobiology.

To demonstrate that there is an overlap between depression, anxiety disorder, and OCD that is likely to rest on shared brain mechanisms, it will be important to consider evidence for shared genes, shared brain mechanisms, and shared treatment effects. However, the starting point is the obvious simple overlap of morbid phenomena in the acute presentation of the different disorders and the common co-occurrence of full syndromes in the same individuals diagnosed impartially using DSM criteria. The common phenomenology is typically the experience of fear and anxiety across a very wide range of psychiatric diagnoses. Formally, the comorbidity of one diagnosis with another beyond chance is the necessary confirmation of a close phenomenological relationship between them. Thus, the various anxiety disorders are highly comorbid with each other. For instance, using lifetime diagnoses in the US population data, 74.1% of those with agoraphobia, 68.7% of those with simple phobia, and 56.9% of those with social phobia also met criteria for another anxiety disorder.2 In general, OCD cases are more likely to show lifetime incidence of other anxiety disorders than vice versa, because of their greater severity and rarity.3 Depression is a comorbidity common to all. Thus, the mood disorders are strongly comorbid with the anxiety disorders, and vice versa. For example, in analyses of lifetime DSM-III-R diagnoses in US population sample data, 58% of individuals with major depression also met criteria for a comorbid anxiety disorder4; the comorbidity rate was only slightly reduced to 51.2% when 12-month diagnoses were used. Conversely, most individuals with diagnosed anxiety disorders also met criteria for major depression, although comorbidity rates varied widely across disorders.

More recent, community-based estimates of the lifetime morbid risk/12-month prevalence ranked by frequency were: major depressive episode: 29.9%/8.6%; specific phobia: 18.4/12.1%; social phobia: 13.0/7.4%; post-traumatic stress disorder: 10.1/3.7%; generalized anxiety disorder: 9.0/2.0%; separation anxiety disorder: 8.7/1.2%; panic disorder: 6.8%/2.4%; bipolar disorder: 4.1/1.8%; agoraphobia: 3.7/1.7%; obsessive-compulsive disorder: 2.7/1.

From the developmental perspective, the anxiety-mood disorders with the earlier median ages of onset are phobias and separation anxiety disorder (ages 15 to 17) and those with the latest are panic disorder, major depression, and generalized anxiety disorder (ages 23 to 30). Comorbidity between anxiety disorders and depressive disorder are common in community samples in various countries where comparable studies have been conducted.6 In summary, the fact of an overlap between different anxiety diagnoses and themselves as well as with depression diagnoses is beyond dispute.

Much of the evidence to this point is based on categorical groups of individuals with disorders as defined in DSM terms. Of all the psychiatric disorders, the anxiety disorders have always been supposed to be common because they reflect experience that is not much removed from normality. Thus, any model based on pathology should also be testable in the reported subjective dimensional experience of healthy populations. This turns out to be the case. A well-known model of normal emotion proposes a “Big Two” dimensional solution; that is dimensions of Negative Affect and Positive Affect7,8 in which Negative Affect is a general dimension of subjective distress.

Hence, it subsumes fear, anger, sadness, guilt, and disgust. An individual who reports feeling sad is also likely to report substantial levels of anger, guilt, fear, etc. This dimension would inevitably predict and so potentially explain major overlaps in the reported experience of someone with anxiety or depression.

The general Positive Affect dimension predicts that an individual who reports feeling happy and joyful will also report feeling interested, excited, confident, and alert. It is more related (negatively) to sadness than to fear. Thus, anhedonia appears as a potentially defining feature of mood disorder as distinct from anxiety, whereas the general subjective distress dimension is feature shared between anxiety disorders and depression.

Clark and Watson subsequently proposed a third component, somatic tension and hyperarousal (eg, shortness of breath, dizziness) as unique to anxiety.9 Therefore, simple subjective symptoms present in a healthy population tend to confirm both a general factor of psychopathology that is expressed across disorders (distress/negative affectivity) and putative specific traits mapping to depression on the one hand and anxiety on the other.

The presence of these factors, unconfounded by help seeking and other factors that contribute to clinical samples, is helpful in understanding why anxiety disorders are likely both to lump and to split. Furthermore it supports the idea that anxiety disorders in particular are simply extreme expressions of traits present across the whole population.

Inheritance of traits and disorders

Since many behavioral traits and psychiatric disorders are heritable, it follows that the structure of these traits or disorders in populations should follow rules of genetic inheritance like other complex traits like height or weight. Twin studies continue to provide the critical observational design: any genetic condition will be more present in identical (ie, monozygotic or MZ) twins than nonidentical twins (dizygotic or DZ). Indeed, such genetic data in a large study of female twins was the first surprisingly strong evidence for genetic overlap between major depression and GAD.10

For family studies of the anxiety disorders of interest here, odds ratios predicting association of illness in first-degree relatives with the illness status of the proband (ie, whether the disorder was present or absent) were homogeneous across studies for all disorders and ranged from 4 to 6, depending on the disorder.11 This extensive overlap may well be associated with a common risk factor such as neuroticism, which has been shown to predict the onset of the anxiety disorders, OCD, and major depression. Neuroticism, a concept originally attributable to Hans Eysenck's studies of personality structure, is about 40% heritable and has been an independent focus for genetic analysis. The contribution through genetic mechanisms of such a globally identifiable factor, which may be summed up to be the trait for anxious worrying, is of great interest. It may represent the major “internalizing factor” that plausibly underlies this wide range of emotional disorders and the ultimate target for all lumpers. Despite its apparent importance, its status is often attacked as being simply a dilute measure of symptoms and its neurobiology has attracted surprisingly limited attention from investigators (see below).

Splitting: the evidence

While lumping and splitting can be presented as profound alternatives, some etiological features, like neuroticism, may necessarily lump, while others may necessarily split. Furthermore, the structural analysis of symptoms suggests that while some are global as described, others are more specific to individual disorders. Indeed, they define what we mean by specificity. For example, Mineka et al12 proposed an integrative hierarchical model of the anxiety disorders. In this model, each individual syndrome was hypothesized to contain both a common and a unique component. The shared component represented broad individual differences in general distress and negative affectivity. As already discussed it will behave as a pervasive higher-order factor (the lumping factor one might say) that is common to both the anxiety and mood disorders. Hence, it will be primarily responsible for the comorbidity issues that were highlighted earlier. However, in addition, each disorder also includes unique features that differentiate it from all of the others. Thus, anxious arousal assumes a limited role as a specific element in syndromes such as panic disorder; trauma history and flashbacks will define post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and obsessions and compulsions define OCD.

This approach to modeling the disorders can provide a simple practical model for diagnostic evaluation via the general and specific components as shown in Figure 1, from the guidelines of the British Association for Psychopharmacology.13 This proposes initial inquiry to establish anxiety symptoms and the presence or absence of depression. If depression is present its treatment is recommended as primary. If it is not, then specific anxiety diagnoses can be made on the basis of defining specific features, so trauma/flashbacks suggest PTSD as above; obsessions and or compulsions, OCD; worry and rumination, GAD, and so on, as shown.

Figure 1.

Suggested scheme for exploring a suspected anxiety disorder. PTSD, post-traumatic stress disorder; OCD, obsessive-compulsive disorder; GAD, generalized anxiety disorder From ref 13: Baldwin DS, Anderson IM, Nutt DJ, et al. Evidence-based pharmacological ...

Why disorders split

In the case of the disorders here, the age of exposure to exacerbating or precipitating stressors, or their nature, might contribute to the determination of a different phenotype from a common genetic background. Indeed, there is some agreement in the existing literature about why an individual at genetic risk for major depressive disorder (MDD)/GAD gets one or the other phenotype. This is based on observed differences in the nature of the environmental stresses that provoke a diagnosable episode. Thus, loss and humiliation events more often precede MDD and danger events more often precede GAD.14

A more detailed consideration of inheritance of anxiety disorders in male and female twin pairs has suggested further division of genetic factors for agoraphobia at one extreme, the specific phobias at the other, and social phobia intermediate between them. The remaining associations between the disorders are largely explained by a unique environmental factor shared across the disorders and, to a lesser extent, a common shared environmental factor. In the most parsimonious model, there appears to be an agoraphobia-specific genetic factor and unique environmental effects (triggers) for each disorder. So, individual genetic factors derive from sets of genes that increase risk for generalized-agoraphobic anxiety on the one hand and specific phobias on the other. Risk across all of the anxiety diagnoses appears to be further increased by life experiences either shared with other family members or unique to the individual. The impact of these life experiences will depend on the disorder. Added to this may be a set of unique environmental factors that increase the liability for one anxiety disorder independent of any other. This is a complicated story, and it is limited by uncertainties of how far subjective reports and recall in a diagnostic interview may shape the findings.15 However, the state of the art is now consistent enough to support the model and suggest the ways in which larger genetic studies may inform our understanding of the molecular underpinnings of emotional behavior.

Molecular genetics

The hard finding of recent years is that success in molecular genetic studies of psychiatric disorder has only come in highly heritable conditions with enormous samples. For the anxiety disorders, there is some suggestion that neuroticism or other broad risk dimensions may provide a tractable target, but we remain a long way from confidence that genetic biomarkers will be of practical value in understanding mechanisms. In the case of neuroticism, there was an early start with the apparent success of candidate gene analysis based on the genetic variation 5-HTTLPR on the serotonin transporter gene SLC6A4 and an apparent link to individual differences in neuroticism. The implicated polymorphism influences mRNA expression so carriers of the short variant (s-allele) appear to show higher neuroticism and also lower mRNA expression of this gene.

The link with serotonin and the role of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) in treating anxiety and depression gave this finding a compelling suggestion of face validity. It could be said that it was a finding that the field yearned to be found correct. Unfortunately, the subsequent findings with respect to 5-HTTLPRand personality traits are heterogenous and have led to a sequence of meta-analyses of the accumulating data, which have led to opposing conclusions. However, there is no doubt that the actual effect size of any association is much smaller than originally believed, if it is real at all.

In the development of this story, the 5-HTTLPR gene was also implicated as mediating an important gene-environment interaction. It was claimed that the s-allele was associated with a substantially greater probability of depressive illness in response to adverse life events. This study is instructive in a number of ways. It was, if possible, an even more attractive finding than the link with neuroticism and appeared to illustrate both the importance of heritability and a gene x environment interaction. It has been massively cited in consequence.18 However, it has also come to exemplify the problems for genetic analysis of behavioral traits and psychiatric disorder more generally.19 Crucially, efforts to replicate the finding and critical analysis of the accumulating findings have led to the conclusion that if the effect is real it is again of much smaller size than originally proposed. The idea that it was a major gene effect that could lead to the development of animal models relevant to anxiety and mood disorder seems in retrospect to have been far too over-optimistic. Instead, it illustrates the general inference that a single common genetic variant when associated with a complex behavioural phenotype will contribute a trivial fraction of phenotypic variance (typically <0.1%).

More recently, genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have permitted testing of about 2 million genetic loci at a time and they have confirmed that the effects of common genetic variants on psychiatric disorders are very small and so require very large sample sizes to be detected. While this has led to success in reliably identifying numerous loci associated with severe disorders such as schizophrenia,20 the sample sizes have not yet grown sufficiently large to yield meaningful results for depression and anxiety. However, it is already clear that there is substantial common variance between the loci identified for schizophrenia and those for bipolar disorder and major depression. The preliminary analyses suggest that the key pathways implicated by these associations relate to synaptic function, immune and neuronal/neurotrophic pathways and histone methylation.21

Go to:

The neurobiology of the amygdala

Notwithstanding the challenge of understanding its genetic foundations, the neurobiology of anxiety (and depression) is of considerable interest. If we are looking for a structure or mechanisms that may provide the experimental focus for understanding the general mechanisms identified from psychopathology, then few look further than the amygdala and its connections. The amygdala is located bilaterally within the medial temporal lobe of higher animals.

It forms a complex extended structure with multiple sub-nuclei. In the rat, the nuclei are divided into three main groups: the basolateral complex, which includes the lateral nucleus, the basal nucleus, and accessory basal nucleus; the cortical nucleus, which includes the lateral olfactory tract; and the centromedial nucleus. The connections give important support to theories of its key functional role. Thus, there is input from all sensory systems. In the case of olfactory, somatosensory, gustatory, and visceral centres the afferent input is from primary sensory structures to the lateral basal and central nuclei. Rather differently auditory and visual information appears to originate in association rather than primary sensory cortex, implying more organized informational content. The amygdala's outgoing pathways project to cortex, hypothalamus, and brain stem; they are potentially targeted to relevant behavioral and neuroendocrine systems.

The central anatomical position of the amygdala within the so-called limbic system or emotional brain was widely accepted by the mid 20th century, before functional studies had investigated how it functioned in this role. It continues to occupy an important position for function, even now when confidence in the usefulness of a limbic/ emotional/archaic brain system has greatly diminished with the recognition of a key cognitive role for the hippocampus.

The function of the amygdala in animals is understood from its established role in fear conditioning. Classical conditioning is a type of learning in which an emotionally neutral conditioned stimulus (CS), often a tone, is presented in advance but predictive of an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US), typically an electric shock to the foot of the animal.

After one or more pairings, the previously emotionally neutral stimulus (CS) elicits a constellation of species-specific conditioned responses (CRs) that are taken to be characteristic of fear, such as freezing or escape behavior, autonomic responses (elevated heart rate and blood pressure), potentiated acoustic startle to aversive acoustic stimuli, and increased neuroendocrine responses (release of stress hormones).

Fear conditioning can be seen as an important adaptive way in which new threats are quickly learned and behavioral responses activated for self-protection. The emergence of a completely neutral stimulus as a potent CS also suggests ways in which the system might fail and give rise to anxiety disorder. Thus a false association of innocuous chance stimuli with threat might be misleadingly incorporated into exaggerated behavioral psychopathology (and explain pathological anxiety). It was an early suggestion that individuals at risk of anxiety disorders would either condition more easily than controls or extinguish fear responses more slowly. In fact only in the last decade has fear extinction in particular come to be widely studied as a translational model for neuroscience.

Numerous studies, employing lesions or electrophysiological recordings, have demonstrated the amygdala's central place in classical conditioning. Thus, lesions to the amygdala impair the acquisition and expression of conditioned fear in rats. The basolateral complex of the amygdala is a potential substrate for the complex sensory convergence from both cortical and subcortical areas required for OS-US association during fear conditioning. In fact, its cells appear to encode the signal by long-term potentiation of EPSPs evoked in the basolateral complex. On the output side, the central nucleus of the amygdala may act as the common pathway to hypothalamus for the generation of fear-conditioned responses.

In man, damage to the amygdala, or areas of the temporal lobe that include the amygdala can be studied occasionally in suitable single cases. One patient with a rare congenital disease that results in the bilateral degeneration of the amygdala was exposed to live snakes and spiders, toured a “haunted” house, and was shown emotionally harrowing films. There was no experience of fear reported, and when she underwent fear conditioning with either visual or auditory CSs and a loud noise as the US, she showed no evidence of fear conditioning (as measured by galvanic skin response). However, her recall of events associated with the fear conditioning procedure was intact. These data support the hypothesis that the amygdaloid complex plays a key role in the acquisition of fear conditioning (whereas the hippocampus is important in remembering the conditioning context).22

Experimental neuropsychology has been transformed, along with the rest human/cognitive neuroscience, by noninvasive brain imaging. With respect to emotional learning, early fMRI studies sought to determine the extent to which rodent models of the amygdala were valid in the human brain. Using a simple differential fear-conditioning paradigm in healthy humans (a blue square as the CS and a mild shock as the US), amygdala activation increased in response to the CS+ (CS that is paired with the US) as compared with the CS- (CS that is not paired with the shock).23 Subsequent fMRI studies using subliminal presentations of fearful faces as stimuli also showed significant amygdala activation in healthy humans.24 These observations provided unequivocal evidence that amygdala function was conserved across species, and validated the use of fMRI for studying fear learning in humans. Accordingly, the amygdala and its connectivity provide a key target for understanding the anxiety disorders and their treatment.

Go to:

Neuroimaging and gene function

Neuroimaging has also seemed to offer a further advantage: access to measures of brain function that might be intermediate to and more sensitive than illness phenotypes to genetic analysis.

The early observation of a possible link between genetic variation of the 5-HTTLPR gene and neuroticism suggested the hypothesis that there might be a more detectable effect of polymorphism in this gene and amygdala function. This has proved controversial and again highlights general problems for the field. Thus, a recent meta-analysis has indicated that there is a statistically significant but small effect of 5-HTTLPR on amygdala activity.25 However, perhaps more striking was the between-study heterogeneity and the evidence for “excess statistical significance.”

In summary, all the individual published studies have been considerably underpowered to detect the size of effect that is likely to be present, which is smaller than originally thought.

In addition, the retreat to a very small or no effect for genetic variation exactly parallels what was summarized previously for this gene and its association with neuroticism. Therefore the claimed advantage of intermediate phenotypes may also be wrong. Measures of systems level neurocognition with fMRI may be no more or less helpful than the behavioral phenotypes like neuroticism or DSM diagnosis for genetic analysis.


Related Solutions

Pick one of the major psychological disorders we learn about this week (Mood Disorders, Anxiety Disorders,...
Pick one of the major psychological disorders we learn about this week (Mood Disorders, Anxiety Disorders, Posttraumatic Stress Disorder, Dissociative Disorders, Personality Disorders). Given what we know about the different types of therapy, how would you go about treating the disorder? Please be sure to explain your reasoning. Do you feel this type of therapy is generally the best? Why?
List the disorders and features [criteria/description] : anxiety related disorders stress related disorders emotion and mood...
List the disorders and features [criteria/description] : anxiety related disorders stress related disorders emotion and mood disorders eating disorders schizophrenia personality disorders
ANxious disorders are on the rise in today's teens use the biological and sociocultural perspectives to...
ANxious disorders are on the rise in today's teens use the biological and sociocultural perspectives to explain why this is occurring. Focus should be on why these disorders are on the rise today for teens. What is different biologically and sociocultural today than compared with teens in the past?
Qustion 1) List 4 mood disorders and 4 personality disorders. 2) Describe the principles of 12...
Qustion 1) List 4 mood disorders and 4 personality disorders. 2) Describe the principles of 12 step treatment approach to substance abuse. 3) List 3 effective treatments for clients with eating disorders
As a nurse who is taking care of patients with mood disorders, what are the significant...
As a nurse who is taking care of patients with mood disorders, what are the significant nursing implications if patient/patients is/are taking monoamine oxidase inhibitors(MAO) , serotonin-norepinephrine uptake inhibitors (SNRIs) and tricyclic antidepressant(TCA) ?
Most admit that there is a stigma associated with mood disorders. Many persons implicitly (and sometimes...
Most admit that there is a stigma associated with mood disorders. Many persons implicitly (and sometimes explicitly) presume that mood disorders occur only in persons who are weak or who "enjoy being sad". Discuss the effect such attitudes might have on persons with mood disorders (reluctance to admit they have a problem or to seek help). what are some ideas to educate the public about causes of these disorders, thus alleviating the stigma associated with them?
In Chapter 8, we saw that ATP is the currency of the cell, powering almost all...
In Chapter 8, we saw that ATP is the currency of the cell, powering almost all the work carried out in a typical cell. In almost all eukaryotes and aerobic prokaryotes, the majority of this ATP is synthesized from the flow of stored hydrogen ions (protons) in the form of a proton gradient. This week’s forum will explore the universality of proton gradients and provide insight into the role they may have played in the origin of life itself and...
12. Discuss the different symptoms and longitudinal course specifiers for mood disorders. Describe they relate to...
12. Discuss the different symptoms and longitudinal course specifiers for mood disorders. Describe they relate to the prognosis the treatment of mood disorders.
Mood Disorders Scenario Scenario: Ally is a 25 year old female who presents to the emergency...
Mood Disorders Scenario Scenario: Ally is a 25 year old female who presents to the emergency department via an ambulance after an intentional overdose. Ally reports she took #25, 50mg Benadryl & #10, 100mg Lithium tablets. She reports feeling lightheaded, nauseated and having a constant ringing in her ears. She is unaware of how long ago the overdose occurred. She stated she had text a friend prior to taking the pills and the next thing she remembers is waking up...
21. Compare and contrast the way mood disorders are expressed in child/adolescents and elderly adults. How...
21. Compare and contrast the way mood disorders are expressed in child/adolescents and elderly adults. How might these differences be explained from a biological perspective? From a social perspective?
ADVERTISEMENT
ADVERTISEMENT
ADVERTISEMENT