Assignment
Sensation and Perception: Signal Detection
Psychologists have always been interested in the relationship
between physical stimuli and the cognitive interpretations of the
sensations and perceptions these stimuli produce. This field of
study is called psychophysics. One of the major contributions of
psychophysics is signal detection theory (SDT). The concepts behind
SDT had a very practical beginning. They were first developed as a
way to help the military pick the best sonar and radar operators
during World War II. For example, detection of an enemy plane on a
radar screen requires picking a target “signal” (the enemy plane)
among lots of other extraneous signals (called “noise”) that may
show up on the screen. Humans vary in their ability to do this, and
SDT provides a way to analyze this variability.
From a psychological perspective, SDT is really a way to
factor a human’s decision-making behavior (called “criteria” or
“bias”) into a perception activity that involves sensitivity to a
stimulus. Consider a lab experiment in which a subject wears
headphones and is asked to indicate whether or not a very weak
sound has been presented on a given experimental trial. Typically,
there is ambient background noise (called “white noise”) present on
every trial, so the subject must pick out the sound within the
context of the background noise. The subjects respond “Yes” if the
sound is heard or “No” if it isn’t. The sound could either have
been present on the trial or not. If the sound is present and the
subject decides “Yes”, this is a correct response, called a Hit. If
the sound is present and the subject decides “No”, this is an
incorrect response, called a Miss. If the sound is absent on the
trial and the subject decides “Yes”, this is what is called a False
Alarm. If the sound is absent on the trial and the subject decides
“No”, this is a Correct Rejection.
We could arrange the possibilities from our example in a
simple chart, which is called an SDT table:
Stimulus (Sound) Actually Present?
Subject’s Response
Yes (Signal + Noise)
No (Noise Only)
“Yes, I hear it”
Hit
False Alarm
“No, I don’t hear it”
Miss
Correct Rejection
Now, let’s put some real numbers into our example. Suppose
that there are 100 trials in our experiment, with 50 trials,
randomly selected, in which the sound is present (called a Signal +
Noise trial, because the stimulus sound is presented “on top of”
the background noise) and 50 trials on which the sound is not
present (called a Noise only trial, because the only thing present
is general background white noise). If, on the 50 Signal + Noise
trials, the subject said “Yes” on 40 trials and “No” on 10 trials,
then that subject got 80% (40/50) Hits and 20% (10/50) Misses. Note
that the Hits and Misses are complementary, so if we know the Hit
percentage, we can find the Miss percentage by subtracting the Hit
percentage from 100%. Similarly, if on the 50 Noise only trials,
the subject said “Yes” to 20 trials and “No” to 30 trials, then
that subject made 40% (20/50) False Alarms and 60% (30/50) Correct
Rejections. Once again, the percentages are complementary.
Now let’s see how we can use these concepts to differentiate
the detection abilities of humans. Assume we run both Subject A and
Subject B through our above example of 100 trials, 50 with the
sound present and 50 with it absent. Let’s say Subject A correctly
detects the target sound 25 times, and Subject B correct detects it
17 times. The question is: “Who is doing better?” You might want to
say Subject A since he got more Hits, but the frequency of False
Alarms clearly needs to be factored in. If Subject A has 20 False
Alarms and Subject B has 5 False Alarms, then B is better at
distinguishing the trials in which the sound is present from the
trials in which the sound is absent. Specifically, these results
would seem to indicate that A is pretty much guessing that the
sound is present but is wrong (i.e., exhibits a False Alarm) as
often as right. Subject B is more selective about saying the sound
is detected but rarely says the target sound is there when it is
not. Thus, it could be argued that B is in actually doing better at
the task.
This example suggests that we need a measure of performance
that includes both Hit rates and False Alarm rates in order to
successfully differentiate among the signal detection abilities of
different people. To this end, researchers have developed a measure
of signal detection sensitivity called d’ (pronounced d-prime) that
can be computed for an individual who has participated in an SDT
task. While the derivation of this measurement is well beyond the
scope of PSYC, it is important to understand that the larger the
value of d’ the better the subject is at distinguishing the target
signal from the noise.
The computation of d’ is fairly complex, but I have provided
an Excel spreadsheet for the calculation given Hit and False Alarm
rates. To continue with our example above, Subject A has a
calculated d’ = 0.253. Subject B has a calculated d’ = 0.869. (You
can verify these numbers using the spreadsheet.) This shows that
Subject B, while having a lower Hit rate, is actually the better
overall performer in our SDT task.
The experiment you are to conduct for Assignment #1 (with you
as the subject) measures face recognition abilities of people using
these same SDT principles. Humans have an uncanny ability to
recognize faces of people they have previously seen, and SDT is a
good tool to investigate individual differences in this
ability.
Procedure
Questions/Tasks
1. Explain in a paragraph or two the concept of signal
detection.
2. What is the independent variable(s) in this
experiment?
3. What is the dependent variable in this experiment?
4. Define “Hit”, “Miss”, “False Alarm” and “Correct Rejection”
in the context of this experiment.
5.Using your scored results, construct an SDT table that
summarizes your performance. Your table should have the same format
as the table on the top of page 2 in this handout with the only
difference being that you are to put your performance percentages
in each cell of the table.
6. Using the “d’ Calculator” Excel spreadsheet, find your d’
value and record it here. This is a measure of your “sensitivity”
(i.e., skill) in the face recognition task. While a higher value is
associated with greater sensitivity and skill, it is more useful
for comparative purposes, so you might want to ask your fellow
students in PSYC what their d’ values were – and, of course, brag
if yours is higher than theirs!
7. Consider a radiologist who is in charge of reading lung
x-rays to detect possible cancerous tumors. Using the Presence of
Stimulus as the Presence of Cancer in this case, build an SDT table
(like the one above) that shows the four possible outcomes in
response to reading a patient’s x-ray. Consider the two possible
errors (False Alarm and Miss) that the radiologist could make. In
the context of the task, explain these two errors and comment on
the ramifications of each one. Many medical people would argue that
one of the mistakes has more serious consequences than the other.
Which one? From an SDT perspective, how might you “coach” or
“train” a radiologist in order to minimize the more consequential
mistake?